Seven Things You Should Know About Pain Science

In the previous post I discussed some common back pain myths, such as the ideas that bulging discs, “bad” posture, or lack of core strength are major causes of back pain. As I noted, the evidence just doesn’t support those claims, and this is somewhat surprising and counterintuitive. However, learning some basics of pain science will go a long way towards making this evidence more understandable.

Pain science has learned a great deal in the last fifty years, but most of this information has had seemingly little impact on the way pain is commonly treated. If you have pain, this is stuff you should know.  By the time you are done reading this post you will know more than many medical providers about pain mechanisms, and maybe even feel a little better as a result, because research shows that pain education can improve outcomes. Here are some basics ideas of pain science.

1.  Pain is a Survival Mechanism whose Purpose is to Protect the Body

Pain is defined as an unpleasant subjective experience whose purpose is to motivate you to do something, usually to protect body parts that the brain thinks (rightly or wrongly) are damaged. If you feel pain, it means that your central nervous system (“CNS”) thinks the body is under threat, and that something has to be done about it.  In this sense, pain is a survival mechanism of fundamental importance. People born without the ability to feel pain (yes, they really exist) don’t live very long. Your CNS takes its job of creating pain very seriously, and therefore you can expect that when it thinks a part of the body is being damaged, it will err on the side of giving you a clear incentive to do something about it.

2.  Pain is an Output of the Brain, Not an Input from the Body

This is the fundamental paradigm shift that has recently occurred in pain science. Pain is created by the brain, not passively perceived by the brain as a preformed sensation that arrives from the body. When a body part is damaged, nerve endings send a signal to the brain containing information about the nature of the damage. But no pain is felt until the brain interprets this information and decides that pain would be a good way to encourage you to take action that will help protect and heal the damage. The brain considers a huge amount of factors in making this decision and no two brains will decide the same thing. Many different parts of the brain help process the pain response, including areas that govern emotions, past memories, and future intentions. Therefore, pain is not an accurate measurement of the amount of tissue damage in an area, it is a signal encouraging action. When a professional musician hurts his hand, his brain might consider very different actions than a soccer player with the same injury. And therefore you can believe that he may get a very different pain response.

3.  Physical Harm Does not Equal Pain.  And Vice Versa.

If you are in pain, you are not necessarily hurt. And if you are hurt, you will not necessarily feel pain. A very dramatic example of tissue damage without pain occurs when a solider is wounded in battle, or a surfer gets an arm bitten off by a shark. In these situations, there is a good chance the victim will not feel any pain at all until the emergency is over. Pain is a survival mechanism, and in cases where pain makes survival even harder, we shouldn’t be surprised that there is no pain. Although most of us have never had our arms bitten off by sharks, we have likely experienced bumps or falls during a sports match or some other minor emergency that we didn’t feel until the game was over. Further, many studies have shown that large percentages of people with pain free backs, shoulders and knees have significant tissue damage in these areas that can be seen on MRI, such as herniated discs and torn rotator cuffs.

How can you have damage without pain? Because for some reason the brain doesn’t think that the damage calls for action. One possible explanation is that the damage occurred slowly over a long period of time in a way that the brain did not find threatening, or maybe the brain just figured the damage was healed as well as possible, and concluded that pain no longer served a useful function. If no action is useful or necessary, or if the action has already been taken, then there is no reason for pain. Have you ever gone to the doctor for pain that disappeared as soon as you walked into the doctor’s office? Perhaps this is the result of the brain relaxing after concluding that the action signal has been heard and that corrective action has been taken.

On the other hand, many people suffer from pain when there is no tissue damage at all. There is a horrible condition called allodynia, where even normal stimuli such as lightly touching the skin can cause excruciating pain. This is an extreme example of something that might occur quite commonly on a much smaller scale – the brain misinterprets innocuous sensory information as evidence of tissue damage, and causes unnecessary pain.

4.  The Brain Often “Thinks” the Body is in Danger Even When It Isn’t

The most dramatic example of this is phantom limb pain, when the victim feels pain in a missing body part. Although the painful limb has been gone for years and can no longer send signals to the brain, the part of the brain that senses the limb remains, and it can be mistakenly triggered by cross talk from nearby neural activity. When this occurs, victims might experience incredibly vivid and painful sensations of the missing limb. Amazingly, phantom arm pain can sometimes be cured by placing the remaining hand in a mirror box in a way that tricks the brain into thinking the missing arm is alive and well!  This is an extraordinary demonstration of the fact that the true target for pain relief is often the brain, not the body.

There are many other more commonplace instances where the brain does not know what is going on in the body and causes pain in an area that is clearly not under threat. Any kind of referred pain, where pain is felt a distance from the actual problem is an example of this. Allodynia is another example.

5.  Pain Breeds Pain

One unfortunate aspect of pain physiology is that the longer pain goes on, the easier it becomes to feel the pain. This is a consequence of a very basic neural process called long term potentiation, which basically means that the more times the brain uses a certain neural pathway, the easier it becomes to activate that pathway again. It’s like carving a groove through the snow while skiing down a mountain – the more times the same path is traveled the easier it is to fall into that same groove. This is the same process by which we learn habits or develop skills. In the context of pain, it means that the more times we feel a certain pain, the less stimulus is required to trigger the pain.

6.  Pain Can Be Triggered By Factors Unrelated to Physical Harm

You may have heard the phrase that neurons that fire together wire together.  The most famous example of this principle is Pavlov’s experiment where he rung a bell each time his dogs ate dinner, then later found that he could cause the dogs to salivate at the mere sound of the bell. What happened at the neural level is that the neurons for hearing the bell became wired to the neurons for salivating, because they fired together consistently for some time. The same thing can happen with pain. Let’s say that every time you go to work you engage in some stressful activity such as working on a computer or lifting boxes in a way that causes back pain. After a while your brain will start to relate the work environment to the pain, to the point where you can start feeling the pain just by showing up, or maybe even just thinking about work. It is no surprise that job dissatisfaction is a huge predictor of back pain.

Further, it has also been shown that emotional states such as anger, depression, and anxiety will reduce tolerance to pain. Although it is hard to believe, research provides strong evidence that a significant portion of chronic back pain is caused more by emotional and social factors than actual physical damage to tissues. You may have noticed that when you return to a place you haven’t been for many years, you quickly fall back into old patterns of speech, posture or behavior that you thought you had left behind permanently. Pain can be the same way, getting triggered or recalled by certain social contexts, feelings or thoughts that are associated with the pain. Ever notice that your pain went away went you went on vacation and came back when you returned?

7.  The CNS Can Change its Sensitivity Level to Pain

There are numerous mechanisms by which the CNS can increase or decrease its sensitivity to a stimulus from the body. The most extreme example of desensitization occurs during an emergency situation as described above, when pain signals from the body are completely inhibited from reaching the brain.

Most of the time an injury will increase the level of sensitization, presumably so that the brain can more easily protect an area that is now known to be damaged. When an area becomes sensitized, we can expect that pain will be felt sooner and more strongly, so that even normally innocuous mechanical pressures can cause pain. There are many complicated mechanisms by which the level of sensitivity is increased or decreased which are far beyond the scope of this article to address. For our purposes, the key point is that the CNS is constantly adjusting the level of volume on the pain signals depending on a variety of factors. For whatever reason, it appears that in many individuals with chronic pain, the volume has simply been turned up too loud and left on for too long.  This is called central sensitization, and it probably plays at least some role in many chronic pain states. It is another example of how chronic pain does not necessarily imply continuing or chronic harm to the body.

Conclusion

When the body is working well, damaged tissues will heal to the best extent possible in a few weeks or months, and then pain should end. Why should it continue if the body has already done its best to heal it?  When pain continues for long periods of time without any real source of continuing harm or damage, there might be a problem with the pain processing system, not the body. Put another way, if you have chronic pain, there is at least some chance that you are not really hurt. Research shows that for some people this is a comforting thought, and serves to reduce anxiety and stress and threat that makes pain worse.

So what else can we do with this info to help get out of pain? The bottom line is that we need to figure out what is causing the CNS to feel threatened and how can we reduce the threat. In the next post I’ll discuss some specific strategies based around movement.

36 Responses to “Seven Things You Should Know About Pain Science”

  1. Does absence of pain indicate absence of injury?

    I lost a fair bit of cartilage in my left knee over the years playing basketball. This was confirmed by constant pain and an MRI. I had microfracture surgery performed, but the surgeon figured it was a temporary stopgap procedure – my lesion was just too big to be fully corrected by simple microfracture. He told me I’d probably need an autologous chondrocyte implantation, which is an invasive procedure designed to embed new living cartilage in the affected area, or risk certain knee replacement surgery in a few years.

    I declined the surgery and have been exercising pain-free for about two years now. I squat, deadlift, sprint, hike, even play basketball, without any pain. I supplement with Vit D (or get lots of sun), Vitamin K2, and follow a paleo eating plan. No grains, sugar, seed oils – the basics. Obviously I’m not necessarily out of the woods yet, but I wonder if my lack of pain indicates some sort of recovery or a deadening of my pain receptors.

    Any thoughts?

    • Erik,

      Thanks for the comments, interesting story, you are a good example of what my post was about. Absence of pain does not indicate absence of tissue damage, but I do think it almost always indicates absence of problem to worry about. In my recent post I talked about the many ways that the pain alarm system can fail us by getting ramped up and start causing pain even without injury. I think is much less likely that the system will fail us by not alerting us to an injury that needs time to heal. I think the system is much more likely to err on the side of creating too much pain and not too little. So, if it doesn’t hurt, don’t worry about it! Of course be mindful that the knee might have a harder time taking extreme work than a younger one but if you just listen to your body I don’t think it will lie to you. Apparently your system is just like the many others that were measured in the above-referenced studies – an injury heals, objective damage remains, but function returns and pain is gone – exactly the way it’s supposed to work.

  2. One last thing, sorry:

    I also wonder if indeed significant structural damage remains, are the lack of pain and lack of immobility indications that the structural damage is no longer a problem?

    I guess I’m unclear whether structural/tissue damage is necessarily a bad thing. Pain may not exist now, and I may retain mobility and function, but will it manifest as something worse down the line?

    Interesting to think about. I suppose I’ll just have to wait and see, eh?

  3. Don’t know if “pain does not originate in the body, it originates in the brain” is a semantic shuffle or what. The signals of inflammation, damage, proprioception, etc. come from the CNS sending them up the spinal cord to the brain. I think how the brain processes that signal depends on where it’s processed in the brain. Lower-level functions are more ingrained and can’t be overridden much by the higher level functions. Eventually, the “bladder is full” message will win out no matter how much you try to hold it. The “my low back hurts when I do this” message can be ignored until the nervous system makes the signal louder because more damage is happening. Or the nerves arborize and grow larger to make a chronic neurogenic pain signal louder.

    Neurogenic “pain” is a pain signal generated by the nerves themselves and does not originate in the brain. You might want to investigate Don Hazen and Jon Martine’s classes on Nerve Mobilization. They may change your ideas about pain and it’s origins.

    • Michael,

      Your comment argues against an idea that does not appear in my blog, and in fact you have misquoted me. I never said that: “pain does not originate in the body, it originates in the brain.” Instead, I said that pain signals from the body do not result in pain until the brain interprets them and decides to create pain. This is a completely different idea. I’m not saying that pain cannot initiate in the body with nociception. Obviously it usually does, and this idea is specifically discussed in the post.

      I’m aware of Hazen’s and Martine’s work, but haven’t attended their classes. My guess is that they would not present a much different picture of nerve mechanics and pain science than that already described by Shacklock, Butler, Mosely, Brieg, Butler, Wall, Melzack, etc. If you would like to read several posts I wrote that are based on these authorities, you can search for the posts called Nerve Mechanics part I to III.

  4. Todd,

    With due respect, regarding your remark. “pain signals from the body do not result in pain until the brain interprets them and decides to create pain,” the statement is true but not very useful. The world doesn’t exist until the brain interprets it. But you don’t fix the paint job on your car by working on your brain, at least not generally.

    • Don,

      Thanks for commenting. I read some of your articles a while back and enjoyed them.

      You said: “With due respect, regarding your remark. “pain signals from the body do not result in pain until the brain interprets them and decides to create pain,” the statement is true but not very useful.”

      I’m not sure why you don’t find the statement useful. You have admitted it is true, and truth is generally useful, particularly when it corrects a misconception. Most people, and indeed most therapists, think that pain equals tissue damage, and this is undoubtedly false. Gaining a better understanding of the brain’s role in pain will lead many therapists or clients to reconsider their approaches to dealing with chronic pain. I will explain some strategies for targeting the brain in my next post.

      As to your analogy about the car, it would be a better analogy to chronic pain is we assume the possibility that the car’s paint job is fine, but only looks bad because the owner for some reason cannot see it clearly, perhaps because he is wearing sunglasses, or is colorblind, or is just depressed and cannot see anything but grey. If that was the case, you wouldn’t advise the person to keep repainting the car, you would try to get them to see the car more clearly. Similarly, when someone has chronic pain because of problem with the brain and not the body, I want to make the brain the target for my therapy, not the body.

    • Don,

      The body image and body schema are fundamental brain maps that are implicated in pain and can be worked on without focusing on the peripheral nerves. If the peripheral nerves are something to be addressed, which is possible, it is processed by the brain through instinctive, emotional, and/or volitional means – nerves can not be coerced by a practitioner if the brain doesn’t give the okay and this takes into account all of the circumstances involved — and it’s worth mentioning that nocioception isn’t a pain signal, per se, and that most people live with it all the time and don’t have pain!

      One more thought, the paint job analogy falls short, IMO, because human’s have a brain that allows for the mapping of space or in other words we CAN paint the proverbial car with our brains and if fact, the notion of ideomotor activity and feedforward processes do just that on a constant basis….

      Reg

  5. Todd,

    Can a persons brain be in a mode of constant desensitization of pain?

    I never take any medications except for the rare occasion of a bad headache, I notice that when the pain reliever wears off, my whole body, at what I perceive to be inside of my bones, hurts for two days afterwards.

    Is it probable that my brain is naturally alleviating this pain through desensitization?

    Or do you think it is an adverse reaction to the pain reliever?

    If not could my brain be perceiving that something is wrong with my bones?

    I do not use any particular brand or type of pain reliever, just whatever is available at the time.

    • Patrick,

      I’m sorry to hear about that. I have never heard of such a problem, and I cant imagine why it would be happening. It sounds like you are having some sort of reaction to your pain relief medication. On the other hand, if you are getting the same reaction from different meds, then why? I would talk to a doc. Good luck.

  6. Good job Johanna! Seems like you’re in school again…. Very interesting read.

  7. Great job with the site, Todd!Love the article, love the website design too.

  8. If my injury is as healed as it’s going to get then I don’t need the pain any more and any regime that will reduce or eliminate it is fine by me. At a very simple level I have found that engaging with the pain and actually saying out loud ‘Thank you for that information, there is no need to provide it again’ is surprisingly helpful.

  9. One of the primary concerns I have, although I agree with everything you say, is that the body is not just a function of the brain. Although the brain has ultimate control over many aspects, it is ultimately stuck with the many conduits that lead to it, particularly the spinal cord and peripheral nerves. We can’t stop our reflexes as much as we can’t hold our breath for too long. In the case of chronic pain, where consistent peripheral nociceptor stimulation causes ongoing postural and reflexive behaviours, we become stuck, often in ‘maladaptive’ cycles that promotes further dysfunctional postural and reflexive behaviours. You can get the brain to ignore it for all you want, downregulate the inputs from the dysfunctional area, but the body will not ignore itself. One thing to note is that the brain interprets abnormal/excessive input from the periphery as harmful, not necessarily damage. It’s not only the brain, you have to also focus on the peripheries.

Trackbacks/Pingbacks

  1. Pain Relief Options: Natural and Medical | Medical News for your Mind, Body and Soul - July 1, 2010

    […] Seven Things You Should Know About Pain Science « Better Movement […]

  2. 07.27.10 | project:deathproof - July 27, 2010

    […] Seven things you should know about pain science […]

  3. Strategies to Reduce Chronic Pain, Part One « Better Movement - August 4, 2010

    […] the previous post I discussed the fact that chronic pain is sometimes not so much a problem with actual ongoing […]

  4. More on Back Pain Myths: Core Strength « Better Movement - August 10, 2010

    […] be the nervous system, which processes and controls pain.  Here are some posts on how that process works and what can be done with that information.  Thanks to Paul for pointing this out. Possibly […]

  5. Strategies to Reduce Chronic Pain, Part Two « Better Movement - September 7, 2010

    […] basic strategies to reduce chronic pain, based on what we know about pain science.  As discussed here, chronic pain is sometimes not so much a problem with actual ongoing physical damage to the body, […]

  6. The Thermal Grill Illusion « Better Movement - September 28, 2010

    […] actual tissue damage to the body.  Put another way, errors in the way the brain maps the body can cause threat which causes pain, even when there is no damage at the site of […]

  7. Pain and Exercise – Knowing when to suck it up - YewHealth - November 14, 2010

    […] Pain is an evolutionary response that evolved to get you out of physical harm. Unfortunately, it doesn’t work all too well. […]

  8. The Monday Effect, or Why Your Massage Lasted for Two Days « Better Movement - December 8, 2010

    […] readers of this blog know, I believe that many problems with chronic pain or physical performance are related to problems with the body maps. For example, as seen above, […]

  9. How to Improve Proprioception « Better Movement - December 29, 2010

    […] smudges, or or other inaccuracies in the body maps can be a significant contributing factor in many chronic pain conditions, and that fixing these problems is a potential way to cure […]

  10. More Back Pain Myths: MRI Abnormalities « Better Movement - February 10, 2011

    […] more evidence that pain lives in the brain not the […]

  11. A Skeptical Look at the Theories of Dr. John Sarno « Better Movement - February 16, 2011

    […] which compare the ideas of Dr. John Sarno to some new pain science concepts that I discussed here. While there may be some superficial similarities, there are some very important differences that I […]

  12. Sunday 2/20/11 « Derby City CrossFit – Louisville, KY - February 19, 2011

    […] 2/20/11 WOD Rest CF Football Here CF Endurance Here Pain Lives in the Brain; Not the Body Everything You Need to Know, Right Here 5 Training Partners; 5 Lessons February 19th, 2011 | […]

  13. Is the “Mind Body Connection” New Agey or Scientific? | Better Movement - February 21, 2011

    […] or have significant effects on various measures of health. Long term emotional stress is a killer. Pain is in the brain. So of course thoughts and feelings can affect the […]

  14. Svens treningslogg - April 16, 2011

    […] […]

  15. More on Pain and Illusions | Better Movement - April 19, 2011

    […] of the main themes of this blog is that therapies attempting to treat chronic pain should target the brain for change, not just the body. Some recent studies based around sensory […]

  16. Should You Stretch a Sore Muscle? | Better Movement - May 12, 2011

    […] the nervous system has concluded that stretching it is threatening to the safety of the body. The pain and stiffness are essentially protective mechanisms – ways to discourage you from lengthening it outside the perceived range of […]

  17. Both Sides Now | Better Movement - September 25, 2011

    […] don’t have any studies showing that unilateral interventions to address pain or coordination will have contralateral effects. However, I suspect that there are some relevant […]

Leave a comment